FAIR USE NOTICE. This document contains copyrighted material whose use has not been
specifically authorized by the copyright owner. The CHANCE project is making this
material available as part of our mission to promote critical thinking about statistical issues. We believe that this constitutes a `fair use' of the copyrighted material
as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law. If you wish to use this
copyrighted material for purposes of your own that go beyond `fair use', you must
obtain permission from the copyright owner.
Copyright 1996 The Sunday Telegraph Limited
Sunday Telegraph
March 31, 1996, Sunday
SECTION: Pg. 20
LENGTH: 587 words
HEADLINE: FOCUS - THE BEEF CRISIS: Why we won't take the risk Robert Matthews, Science
Correspondent, on how the experts misread the public mood
BYLINE: By ROBERT MATTHEWS
BODY:
IT WAS with some exas peration that Professor John Pattison, chairman of the Government's
panel of experts on BSE, reiterated his considered scientific view on the risk of
contracting CJD from eating beef. In all normal senses of the word, he declared, eating beef is safe. Yet with the intransigence of a surly bull, the British
public and foreign governments remain unmoved. On the face of it, the explanation
is simple: most people are simply too thick and irrational to understand the concept
of risk. They gormlessly fret about the dangers of eating meat while driving along
in cars responsible for 5,000 deaths a year. But some experts were last week
pointing the finger of blame for the current crisis at Prof Pattison and his
colleagues, and their ignorance of a complex field of research: the assessment of risk.
By failing to understand how people assess risk, the panel of experts - and the
politicians they advise - may have cost thousands of people their livelihoods,
perhaps even their lives. Part of the blame lies in the failure of Prof Pattison and his
colleagues to provide a basis for their statements. Why do they believe eating
beef is safe? The answer lies in simple probability theory: the more independent
factors that are involved in an event - like death through eating meat - the less likely
that event becomes. In the case of beef, there are five (and possibly more)
such factors: The animal must carry BSE - and most beef cattle never have. That
animal must be sent to an abattoir for slaughter - which, given that farmers now
get full compensation, has become a relatively rare event. Infected parts from
the animal must find their way into human food - which again the Government has
taken steps to prevent by banning certain offals. Those infected parts must survive food
processing: tinning and roasting is likely to destroy any prion protein. Whatever
infection remains must be able to affect humans. And, despite the fuss over the
10 patients with an apparently new form of CJD, the evidence for that link remains circumstantial.
Indeed, experiments with genetically engineered mice currently suggests there
is no link. Last week, Dr Helen Grant, a retired neurologist, added another possibility: that the 10 cases may simply be the result of a rare genetic susceptibility
to CJD. What the scientists cannot do at present is put hard figures to all
these probabilities, especially the final one. What is clear, however, is that "revelations" about one element - such as proscribed offal reaching the human food chain
- are hardly cause for blind panic. Yet according to Professor Lawrence Phillips,
an expert on risk assessment at the London School of Economics, even if the risks of meat eating could be quantified - say, one in a million - it would not make
the slightest difference to public confidence. "The reason is that scientists
and the public mean different things by risk," he said. "For the scientist, it
is the probability of something unpleasant happening, while for the public it is
something much more complex." Research into risk perception by American
psychologists has shown that most people base their risk assessments on three
key factors: dread, lack of knowledge, and the number of people involved. "Probabilities
play no role at all, and the BSE issue scores high on all three of these factors,"
said Prof Phillips. "Instead of saying the public doesn't understand risk, they ought to get informed about the way the public thinks about risk."