!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
Chance News 5.11
(8 September 1996 to 8 October 1996)
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
Prepared by J. Laurie Snell, with help from Bill Peterson,
Fuxing Hou, Ma.Katrina Munoz Dy, and Joan Snell, as part of the
CHANCE Course Project supported by the National Science
Foundation.
Please send comments and suggestions for articles to
jlsnell@dartmouth.edu.
Back issues of Chance News and other materials for teaching a
CHANCE course are available from the Chance web site:
http://www.geom.umn.edu/locate/chance
=====================================================
My only hope is that at least it may stagger
you in your certainties.
Darwin
=====================================================
Contents Part 1
Contents Part 2
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Part 1
Alan Levine suggested the following segment from Jim Lehrer's
Sept. 25 PBS news program.
JIM LEHRER. Frequent drug use among teenagers is on
the increase according to a report issued today. It
was based on a survey by an Atlanta-based group called
PRIDE, the Parents Resource Institute for Drug
Education. The executive director said the use of
most drugs was at the highest levels in nine years.
DOUG HALL, Executive Director, PRIDE: A high school
classroom with 30 students--our studies show that 3.5
percent of the 12th grade tried heroin last year. That
means that in every 12th grade classroom in America -
every single classroom - one student, a 17 or 18 year
old, had already tried heroin. Two had tried cocaine.
Three had tried amphetamines. And nearly four had tried
LSD, PCP, or some other hallucinogen.
Hall's first sentence was probably not transcribed
exactly right but the rest of his remarks are exactly as
stated in PRIDE'S official press released on their web site.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) What do you think of Hall's understanding of
statistics?
(2) Do you think that Hall was suggesting that 10 students in
every senior class of 30 students had tried one of the these
drugs?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Emil Freeman and Evan Fisher suggested the following three
related letters to the editor of the "New York Times".
TWA Flight 800 crash, don't discount meteor.
The New York Times, 19 September 1996, A26
Letter to the editor by Charles Hailey and David Helfand
The writers refer to an earlier article about the TWA Flight
800 crash in which it is reported that "more than once,
senior crash investigators have tried to end the speculation
by ranking the possibility of friendly fire at about the same
level as that a meteorite destroyed the jet." They feel that
this must be based on a misconception of the probability that
a meteorite would destroy a jet and write:
The odds of a meteor striking TWA Flight 800 or
any other single airline flight are indeed small.
However, the relevant calculation is not the
likelihood of any particular aircraft being hit,
but the probability that one commercial airliner
over the last 30 years of high-volume air travel
would be struck by an incoming meteor with sufficient
energy to cripple the plane or cause an explosion.
Approximately 3,000 meteors a day with the requisite mass
strike Earth. There are 50,000 commercial airline takeoffs
a day worldwide. Adopting an average flight time of two
hours, this translates to more than 3,500 planes in the
air; these cover approximately two-billionths of Earth's
surface.
Multiplying this by the number of meteors per day and the
length of the era of modern air travel leads to a 1-in-10
chance that a commercial flight would have been knocked
from the sky by meteoric impact.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Do you believe that the authors' numerical assumptions
are reasonable? What other assumptions have the authors have
made in arriving at their "1 in 10 chance"?
(2) Assuming the authors' assumptions to be correct, try to
determine the "chance that a commercial flight would have
been knocked from the sky by meteoric impact". Do you obtain
the same "1 in 10 chance" that the authors did?
(3) Could you estimate the chance that the TWA Flight 800
plane was hit by friendly fire? That any plane in the last
30 years was hit by friendly fire?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Meteor and Plane Crash.
The New York Times, 24 Sept. 1996, A24
Letter to the editor from Guy Maxtone-Graham
Maxtone-Graham writes:
As any statistician can tell you, the outcome of
past, random events has no bearing on future,
unrelated random events. Toss a coin 10 times and
the odds of getting heads or tails on the 11th toss
are still 50-50.
Likewise, calculations based on the number of flights
worldwide, the number of takeoffs per day and the
number of years that commercial flights have thrived
have no bearing on the question of whether a rock
from outer space happened to enter the atmosphere to
hit one particular airliner on July 17. The odds of
such a freak accident downing a specific flight
remain small, and the professors' conclusion that
"the meteor impact theory deserves more considered
attention" is difficult to suppor
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Do you believe there is anything that all statisticians
would agree to?
(2) Do you agree with the argument expressed in this letter?
(3) In what has been called the "streak of streaks", Joe
Dimaggio in 1941 got a hit in 56 successive games. Should
you compute the probability that a typical player would
achieve such a streak or that sometime in the history of
baseball such a streak should occur? How would you estimate
these probabilities?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Meteors and numbers that count.
The New York Times, 28 September 1996, Section 1, page 22
Letter to the editor from Bill Grassman
Attempts to prove or disprove the probability that
TWA Flight 800 was the victim of a meteor recall the
tale of the business executive who, concerned that he
might be on a plane with a bomb, commissioned a study
to determine the odds of that happening.
When the calculations of flights per day, when and
where the bombings had occurred and the normal flying
patterns of the executive disclosed that the odds of
his being on a plane with a bomb were 1 in 13 million,
he asked for the probability of his being on a plane
with two bombs. On learning that this increased the
odds to 1 in 42 billion, he always carried a bomb with
him. Statistics!
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Do you think the writer is criticizing statistics for saying
this is true or criticizing those who misunderstand statistics?
(2) How would you explain to your Uncle George what is wrong
with the executive's reasoning that he should carry a bomb?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Daniel Atlan found the following story on the front page of the
24 Sept. issue of "Le Monde". This story also appeared in the
"Sunday Times".
Eurocrats bank on the stars.
Sunday Times, 22 September 1996, Home News
Nick Gardner and Jonathan Leake
The European Bank for Reconstruction and Development (EBRD) has
been using astrological events to help it predict fluctuations
in financial markets. Astrologers have claimed powerful links
between the fluctuations in the financial markets and celestial
events such as lunar eclipses.
Mark Curtis, the bank's treasurer, said the bank has a duty to
investigate any method which appeared to give it an advantage
when playing the market. The American trader and hypnotist
Robert Krausz has been taken on as a special adviser.
The bank has carried out in-depth computer tests on astro-
economic theories. The system compared past fluctuations in the
money market with events such as lunar eclipses to see if there
was a relationship. One of the most important findings claimed
for the astrological system has been that markets become
destabilised around the time of lunar eclipses. This means that
financiers can buffer themselves against violent movements.
Curtis emphasized that astrology-economics played only a small
part in the banks investment policies.
The bank was founded five years ago to raise capital from its
60-member governments to provide loans to boost eastern European
economies. In 1993 it was dubbed the "glistening" bank after
revelations that it had spent far more on its headquarters in
London than it had given out in loans.
DISCUSSION QUESTION:
What do you think the bank will be dubbed now?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Study finds stunted lungs in young smokers.
The New York Times, 26 Sept. 1996, B10
Jane E. Brody
A study reported in the "New England Journal of Medicine" showed
that lung development is impaired in teen-agers who smoke as few
as five cigarettes a day.
The study involved 5,158 boys and 4,902 girls in six areas in
the United States where environmental pollution was a particular
concern. The children, aged 10 to 18, were given annual exams
from 1974 to 1989. Their lung capacity was measured by seeing
how much air they could get out after taking a deep breath and
blowing out as hard as they could. From these measurements it
was observed that children who smoked had less lung capacity
than those who did not, and the amount less was directly related
to the number of cigarettes smoked and how long they had been
smoking.
The researchers also looked at asthma and wheezing and found
that 25% of the nonsmoking teens had episodes of wheezing as
compared to 56% of the boys who smoked from 5 to 14 cigarettes a
day and 47% of the boys who smoked this many cigarettes a day.
It is stated that the better the lung functions in youth the
healthier the lungs will be in later life.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) The authors concluded that lung damage due to smoking for girls
was greater than it was for boys. What confounding factors might
make it difficult to compare the effect of smoking on the lungs
of boys and girls?
(2) The article states that every day 3,000 adolescents begin to
smoke. Do you think that reporting studies like this one will
help decrease this number?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Incarceration is a bargain.
Wall Street Journal, 23 Sept. 1996,
Steve H. Hanke
The author states that a recent article by economist Steven D.
Levitt (Quarterly Journal of Economics, May 1996) showed that
violent crime would be approximately 70% higher today if our
prison population had not increased since 1973, and property
crime would be almost 50% more frequent. A graph is provided
showing that an increase in prison population reduces all major
categories of violent and non-violent crime. From this chart it is
observed that on average about 15 crimes per year are eliminated
for each additional prisoner locked up.
The author remarks that incarceration works and then asks if it
pays. He quotes results from Levitt estimating that the average
annual cost of incarceration is $30,000 a year while the annual
amount of damage the average criminal would do if on the loose
is $53,000.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) How do you think Mr. Levitt came to the figure that the
average criminal would cause $53,000 damage annually?
(2) Hanke did not include those who are in jail for so-called
consentual crimes such as drug offenders. Drug offenders have
been estimated to be more than half the federal prison
population. Why do you think he omitted these?
(3) How do you think that Levitt estimated how much more
violent crime would have occurred if the prison population had
not increased since 1973?
(4) In Levitt's article it is reported that one way to estimate
the deterrent effect of having people in prison is to ask
prisoners how many crimes they commit when not imprisoned. In a
Wisconsin study this yielded a distribution of non drug-related
crimes per year having median 12 and mean 141. What do you think
the mode of this distribution was? How reliable do you think
such statistics are?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
In the last Chance News I promised a review of another book that
would be a valuable resource for teaching a Chance course or any
introductory statistics course. Here it is:
A casebook for a first course in statistics and data analysis.
John Wiley, 1995
Samprit Chatterjee, Mark S. Handcock, Jeffrey S. Simonoff
This book provides a set of case studies from a variety of
fields where statistical analysis is required to reach a
meaningful decision. Each case describes the background of the
problem, provides the data and poses questions that can be
answered by statistical analysis. The cases fall into three
categories: in the first category the case study is completely
analyzed. In the second category the cases are not analyzed,
but suggestions are made to guide the student in carrying out an
analysis. In the third category the problem is described, the
data provided, questions are posed, and the student is left to
carry out the analysis.
To give a flavor of these case studies we describe the first
one. It deals with eruptions of the "Old Faithful" geyser
at Yellowstone National Park in Wyoming. Visitors visiting
the geyser want to arrive at a time when they will not have
to wait too long to view the geyser. The National Park Service
erects a sign predicting when the next eruption will occur.
How can they best predict the time between eruptions?
The authors provide data giving the time between eruptions and
the duration of eruptions for August 1978 and August 1979. A
histogram of the time between eruptions is found to be bi-model
with peaks at about 55 minutes and 80 minutes. A scatter plot of
time between eruption and duration of eruption shows that long
eruptions tend to be followed by long intervals between
eruptions, and short durations are followed by short times
between eruptions. An explanation for this is given: eruptions
of short duration leave hot water in the ground making it easier
for the water to heat up for another eruption but for long
eruptions most of the hot water in the earth is used up by the
eruption requiring more heating for the next eruption.
The authors are led to a prediction rule that predicts that
after an eruption of duration less than 3 minutes, one will
have to wait about 55 minutes, and after an eruption of duration
greater than 3 minutes the wait will be about 80 minutes. Data
for August 1985 is provided and this rule is used to predict the
time of the next eruption. This prediction is found to be
within plus or minus 10 minutes about 90% of the time.
The authors have made good use of their web site to add new
information about their cases and to provide additional case
studies suggested by later work. For example, you will find
here an analysis of the Old Faithful data by Donald Richter
which shows that the tacit assumption made that the
distributions of eruption times and times between eruptions are
the same in August for different years, may not be completely
justified and should be considered only as an approximation.
Faithful readers of Chance News (see Chance news 5.03) will
recall that researchers at Yellowstone Park reported (The New York
Times Feb 5, 1996, D1) that the average time between eruptions
appears to be increasing through the years. Chatterjee and his
co-authors wrote us about this:
The bi-modility has a direct effect on the question of
lengthening average time intervals between eruptions.
There are two obvious ways that the average time
interval could increase, without changing the basic
underlying pattern of eruptions: by a general shift
upwards of the entire distribution, or by a change in
the probabilities of a short time interval versus a
longtime interval (with long intervals becoming more
probable). Presumably these two different possibilities
would have different implications from a geothermal
point of view; so it would be interesting to see if
either possibility (or what other possibility) describes
the observed lengthening of the average time interval.
Thus we see how the web allows us to keep an interesting
problem alive.
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Bob Griffen reminded us that the great Milwaukee "voucher
battle" is still going on (See Chance News 5.09). Here is a
recent article that describes the present state of this voucher
battle.
Dueling professors have Milwaukee dazed over school vouchers.
Wall Street Journal, 11 October 1996, A1
Bob Davis
Since 1990, Millwaukee families of several thousand low-income
families have been provided state-funded vouchers to allow them
to take their students out of public schools and enroll them in
private schools. The program has been watched closely as a
model program designed to give poor children some of the
advantages of children of wealthier families.
John Witte was selected by the state to track the progress of
the program. In a series of annual reports he compared the
progress of the voucher students to a control group chosen from
the general Milwaukee school population. He found that voucher
students did not advance faster than the control group despite
the fact that the parents of the children felt that the private
school atmosphere was much better for their children.
Harvard political scientist Paul Peterson was critical of
comparing the progress of the voucher students to randomly
chosen Milwaukee students. He carried out his own study by
taking advantage of the fact that four private schools had more
applicants than they had space for and so used a lottery to
decide which to accept. Peterson compared the performance of
those accepted and those not accepted and found that, while
their performance in the first year was no better, it was
significantly better on standardized tests after three years.
The issue has become highly political and, in fact, occurred in
the last presidential debate. Dole is supporting the voucher
plan, promising a $3 billion-a-year federal program to pay for
scholarships to send low-and middle-income children to private
schools. Clinton, while not opposing local voucher programs,
would not support a federal voucher program with the "highly
ambiguous" results in Milwaukee.
Peterson and Witte are carrying out a virtual duel over the
statistical issues involved and you can find the data, the
studies and their critiques of each other's work on the web page
of the American Federation of Teachers.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) In his critique of Peterson's work, Witte writes:
The methodology employed by Peterson is one used
primarily in controlled medical experiments. It is
theoretically inappropriate for modeling educational
achievement and, in its application to the MPCP, is
very biased in favor of choice students. The method,
to my knowledge, has never been used before in modeling
educational achievement.
What do you think Witte means by: "It is theoretically
inappropriate for modeling educational achievement"?
(2) Peterson says about Witte's study:
Mr. Witte stacks the deck against voucher students
by comparing them with Milwaukee students
generally. The latter students are whiter
wealthier and more frequently live in two-parent
families than voucher students--and thus more
likely to perform better on standardized tests.
Mr. Witte's efforts to make adjustments for the
different groups are doomed because they are too
dissimilar. The statistical controls that Mr.
Witte used can't turn middle-income white students
into lower-income blacks.
What do you think of this criticism?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Full House.
Harmony Books
Stephen Jay Gould
It is always fun to find a "best seller" that is full of
statistical concepts. Full House is a popular version of several
of Gould's previous works. Those who teach Chance are familiar
with two of these: Gould's delightful article "The Median is not
the Message" (Discover Magazine June 1985), and Gould's
explanation of why there are no more .400 hitters in baseball
told by him on the "Against All Odds" video.
In the "Discover" article Gould describes how he reacted to
the news he received in 1982 that he was suffering from
abdominal mesothelioma, a rare and serious cancer with a
median mortality of 8 months after it is diagnosed. Gould
describes how he cheered himself up by realizing the
distribution of lifetimes for those diagnosed with his disease
could not have a left tail of more than 8 months and probably
had a very large right tail. He assumed that he was far out
in this tail. Evidently he was.
In "Against All Odds", Gould explained the disappearance the
.400 hitter by observing that, while the average batting average
for the past 100 years remained around .260, the standard
deviation of this average continually decreased. Ty Cobb's .420
batting average in 1911 was 4.16 standard deviations above the
mean batting average and Ted Williams' .406 batting average in
1941 was 4.21 standard deviations above the mean for that year.
Thus while the .400 hitters seemed to disappear after Ted
Williams, because of the decrease in the standard deviation for
the average batting average, George Brett's .390 batting average
in 1980 was 4.03 standard deviations above the mean. Thus his
.390 batting average represented a comparable exceptional
average.
Gould argues that the batters are, in fact getting better; but
so are the pitchers and fielders. So this improvement does not
show up in their batting averages. We should not be measuring
excellence by the batting average but rather by how many
standard deviations the players average is above the mean of the
batting averages. Gould explains the decrease in standard
deviation by the fact that players in every position are getting
near the limit of what is possible so there is less room for
variation.
Thus, the theme of this book is that it is misleading to look at
a single attribute of a system such as maximum values but,
rather, you have to look at the "full house".
In both examples, variation is effected by boundaries that are
imposed. In the first example, Gould's additional lifetime
after being diagnosed must be at least 0. In the baseball
example there are physiological limits to how good the players
can get.
To get into the spirit of his main example, evolution, Gould
looks at a drunkard's walk example. He gives an example of
random walkers something like this. Consider a lot of random
walkers who move on the integers, making one step to the right
or left with equal probabilities. If they ever reach 0 they
disappear (0 is a bar). Start 100 such random walkers at 10.
Then the distribution of their positions after 100 steps will
have mean about 10 but will have a right tail that is much
longer than the left tail (limited to 10). When I carried out a
simulation of this experiment, the mean position was 10.6, the
mode was 10 and the right tail of the distribution extended to
32 achieved by just one of the random walkers.
To show what this means for evolution, Gould considers the case
of single-celled protozoans called planktonic forams. Evidently
these forams occur in fossils, and their evolution has been
studied through millions of years. It has been observed that,
over time, their body size appears to increase. This increase
has been explained by Darwin's concept of "survival of the
fittest" in terms of an advantage for larger bodies. However,
Gould's argues that this does not really fit into Darwin's
scheme of things and the apparent increase in size can be
accounted for even with no tendancy for size to increase by the
existence of a left barrier just as in the case of the random
walkers.
Gould looks at a study done by W.C. Parker at Florida State
University which followed the descendants of 342 species of
forams. The laboratory procedures to study forams sieve out
forma with size less than a certain minimal size. Thus when
following the changes in size of descendants of a given species,
if a change results in a size smaller than this minimum size,
this species will disappear just like our random walker did upon
reaching 0.
Parker showed that the distribution of the changes in sizes from
one generation to the next is symmetric with no preference for
being larger or smaller. In other words these changes looked
like the changes of our random walker with no preference for
going to the right or left. Thus, just as for our random walker,
the mean of the distribution of size should remain about the
same. But as time goes on, the distribution of size will have a
large right tail, making it appear that something is causing
the forams to be getting bigger when, in fact, it is just a
natural consequence of statistical variation in a random process
with a barrier.
Well, from this example it is only one step to conclude that the
same kind of argument can be used to explain the apparent
increase in complexity or intelligence in the human species.
This increase has nothing to do with adapting to our environment
but rather to the fact that there is a limit to how dumb or
simple a species can be but not to how smart or complex it can
become. Thus starting from a bacteria, over a long time,
random variations in species have produced some very intelligent
forms of life. We are lucky enough to be on this right tail of
the distribution of many random walkers. We started as bacteria
and the mode intelligence after all these millions of years of
evolution is still that of bacteria providing Gould further
evidence that he is on the right track.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) A 1992 study of abdominal mesothelioma still gives the
median mortality time to 12, 10, or 8 months, depending on the
state at which it is diagnosed. What might have been the source
of Gould's good fortune?
(2) Gould suggests that, while the mode should stay the same,
the mean would increase in his model for the size of forams.
Should the mean increase if his random model with a barrier is
correct?
(3) From p. 175: `If we could replay the game of life again
and again, always starting at the left wall and expanding
thereafter in diversity, we would get a right tail almost
every time, but the inhabitants of this region of greatest
complexity would be wildly and unpredictably different in
each rendition---and the vast majority of replays would
never produce (on the finite scale of a planet's lifetime) a
creature with self-consciousness. Humans are here by the
luck of the draw, not the inevitability of life's direction
or evolution's mechanism.'
What do you think Gould means by a `vast majority'?
How do you suppose Gould carried out this computation?
Is self-consciousness merely a matter of large brain
capacity?
(4) Look closely at Figure 27 on p. 165, which Gould says
is taken from Stanley's work. Notice that all the big
rightward jumps in the picture originate on the right side
of the picture. Why? If there is a mean tendency
for offspring to increase in complexity, does this
invalidate Gould's central contention (p. 162) that
size increase `is really random evolution away from
small size, not directed evolution toward large size'?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Ask Marylin
Parade Magazine, 15 Sept 1996, p. 24
Marylin vos Savant
Marylin is asked:
Say I have two dozen pickles--labeled No. 1 through
No. 24--in a jar. I pull out a pickle at random,
note the number and replace the pickle in the jar.
Then my friend pulls out a pickle at random. What
are the chances that she will pull out a higher-
numbered pickle than I did?
Paul Joseph, Houston Texas
Marylin observes that the chance that you obtain the same number
is 1/24. If you don't get the same number, you have an equal
chance that your number is smaller or bigger than the first
number. Thus the answer is 23/48. Marylin remarks that her
answer would be different if you tell her the outcome of the
first choice.
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Part 2
NOTE: At the end of this Chance News you will find a call for papers for the Fifth
International Conference on Teaching Statistics - ICOTS-5 - to be held in Singapore,
June 21-26, 1998.
Interesting web sites: COLLEGEBALL.COM
This is a "for fun" football pool. Each week about 18 of the leading college games
are picked. Your instructions are:
Make your picks for each game by clicking on the
radio button next to the team. Assign a unique
confidence value for the pick from 1-18. 1 is the
highest confidence and 18 is the lowest. Use each
confidence value only once!
Point spreads are not used. Suggested strategies for this pool are given. For an
interesting example of strategy in football pools with point spreads, see "The Evil
Twin strategy for a football pool"
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
It's election time and you may want to follow the tracking polls. You can find these
at the following sites:
PoliticsNow
AllPolitics
Gallup
And don't forget our favorite "The Iowa Electronic Markets" where you can play the
futures market for real money. You can buy a share of Dole today for about 8 cents.
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Milt Eisner suggested the next two articles. The full text of these articles can be
found on the Washington Post homepage. They only keep articles there for two weeks.
Tails Over Heads.
Washington Post, 13 Oct 1996, C1
William Casey
Ever since January 1985, William Casey has been collecting all the coins that he sees
on the ground wherever he goes (coins includes bills). He has established a data
base containing information about these coins. In this article he discusses several
aspects of his collection which now contains 11,902 coins. He reports, for example, that
51.5% of the coins were found with tails up. He provides a bar graph showing the
distribution of coins found by month, with January yielding the most and September
the fewest. Casey plans to continue collecting coins until Dec. 31, 1999.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Casey wonders why 51.5 percent of his coins were found tails up. Is this a significant
difference from chance? If so, what explanations might you give him?
(2) Casey also asks: why is the average mint date 1985 for Philadelphia pennies but
only 1983 for those from Denver? What would you have to know to decide if this difference
is significant?
(3) Do you have any idea why January should be the best month for finding coins?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
The next three articles are a series based on a survey by the Washington Post, Harvard,
and the Kaiser Foundation.
A nation that poor-mouths its good times.
Washington Post, 13 Oct 1996, A1
Richard Morin and John M. Berry
This article discusses the difference between the actual performance of the U.S. economy
(as reflected in statistics) and the public's perception of the economy. The article's
title indicates that the former is pretty good while the latter is pretty bad. The authors surveyed 1511 Americans with the following open questions:
- What percentage of US workers are unemployed?
- What is the current inflation rate?
- Is the budget deficit larger or smaller than
it was five years ago and what is the current
deficit?
- Is the gap between the rich and poor bigger
or smaller than it was 20 years ago?
The article reports "according to government statistics, unemployment is at a seven-year
low. Inflation is at its lowest level in three decades. The federal budget deficit
has declined to about $109 billion this year from $290 billion in 1992. The economy is creating millions more jobs than are being lost to downsizing, mergers, business
failures or foreign competitions."
On the other hand, from the survey, we learn that "the average American thinks the
number of jobless is four times higher than it actually is. Nearly 1 in 4 believes
the current unemployment rate tops 25 percent--the proportion of Americans who were
estimated to be out of work is as it was at the worst of the Great Depression. They believe
that prices are rising four times faster than they really are and that the federal
budget deficit is higher, not lower, than it was five years ago. And 7 in 10 say
there are fewer jobs than there were five years ago."
The article discusses problems this disparity makes for politicians creating public
policy.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Do you think the average American should know the answers to the four questions
listed above?
(2) Why do you think there is such a disparity between what the public thinks and
the government reports?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Prosperity imbalance.
Washington Post, 14 Oct. 1996, A1
Clay Chandler and Richard Morin
This second article in the series reports that the survey and other data show that
people are divided into distinct groups: those who have trouble meeting their bills
and see little hope for a life without financial woes and those who are well off
and have no financial problems. The gap between these groups has increased significantly
in recent years. The main factor in deciding who is in each group is education.
The family income of a family with a college diploma increased 28 percent in inflation-adjusted dollars from 1974 to 1994 while incomes of high school graduates increased just
3 percent and incomes of those without a high school diploma shrank more than 10
percent.
Not surprisingly, the people in these two groups have very different attitudes about
the state of the U.S. economy even though, the economy appears to be in good shape.
DISCUSSION QUESTION:
Why do people with a college education fare so much better today than they did thirty
years ago?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Great divide: economists vs. public.
Washington Post, 15 Oct. 1996, A1
Mario A. Brossard and Steven Pearlstein.
This last article in the series compares the opinions of economists about the economy
with those of the general population. Opinions of the economists were obtained from
a survey of 250 economists from a random sample of members of the American Economic
Association. The opinions of the general public were obtained from the Post/Harvard/Kaiser
study.
There is a wide disparity between the opinions of the general public and the economists.
For example: 35% of the economists feel that, in the past 20 years, family incomes
have been going up faster than the cost of living while only 11% of the public believe this. 39% of the economists feel that most of the new jobs being created today
pay well as compared to only 16% of the public who believe they do. 50% of the economists
but only 24% of the public feel the average standard of living will increase over
the next five years. 50% of the economists believe that trade agreements have helped
create more jobs while only 17% of the public believe this.
The article discusses the many reasons for this difference. For example, it is stated
that the public tends to look locally at what is happening in the country and the
economists look globally. The economists see that trade agreements lead to less
expensive goods while the public sees the local cobbler goes out of business.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) How do you think the economic status of the average economists compares with
that of the average person in the general survey? If different, would that explain
some of the differences in their answers?
(2) Should the President, in proposing public policy, be more influenced by the opinions
of the economists or by the opinions of the public?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
College board revises test to improve chances for girls.
New York Times, 2 October, 1996, B8
Karen W. Arenson
In an agreement with the Office of Civil Rights, the College Board will add a multiple-choice
test on writing to its Preliminary Scholastic Assessment Test (PSAT). This test will
include questions involving the structure of language and standard written English. The PSAT test is taken by high school juniors and is the main determinant in
awarding the National Merit Scholarships.
This followed a complaint filed with the Education Department in 1994 by civil rights
activists who said that girls tend to score lower on the PSAT than boys, even though
their high school and college grades were better.
Each year more than a million high school juniors take the PSAT tests. About 55%
of these are girls. Those in the top 15,000 scores, of which about 60% are boys,
automatically become National Merit semifinalists. The students then submit grades,
extracurricular activities, recommendations and essays; and about 14,000 are chosen as finalists.
In 1989, a Federal District Court in Manhattan ruled that the New York Regents Scholarships
discriminated against girls because they were based on SAT scores. When New York
State relied on standardized test, girls won 43 percent of the scholarships. A year after the court ruling, when grades were also taken into consideration, the girls
won 51% of the scholarships.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) A spokesman for the College Board denied that the test in its present form is
biased against women. What does it mean to say that the PSAT test is biased against
women?
(2) A spokesman for the College Board said about the writing test to be added that
"Women tend to do better than men on this test, about two or three points better
on a high score of 800". Do you think that will significantly help the women to get
National Merit Scholarships?
(3) From the 1996 Profile of College-Bound seniors we find that the following statistics:
Verbal SAT Males Females
Mean 507 503
Standard 112 109
Deviation
Percentiles
75th 580 580
50th 510 500
25th 430 430
Math SAT Males Females
Mean 527 492
Standard 115 107
Deviation
Percentiles
75th 610 560
50th 530 490
25th 450 420
These statistics are based on the 1,084,725 students who took these tests any time
during their high school years through March 1996. If the student took it more than
once the most recent score was used.
(1) Why do you think there is such a large difference between the mean math scores
for men and women?
(2) Is there significant less variation in the women's scores than in the mens? If
so, can you explain this?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Use of daily election polls generate debate in press.
New York Times, 4 Oct. 1996, A24
James Bennet
On Saturday the latest CNN/USA Today Gallop tracking poll showed only a 9-point difference
between Clinton and Dole. A CNN commentator reported that "the race has closed. This
single digit lead is half of what it was earlier this month." On Tuesday it showed a 25-points difference, and another CNN reporter stated that "Mr. Clinton's domestic
political standing is strong, he has opened up his largest lead since the poll began
in September."
Sharp changes are newsworthy but are they significant?
These daily reports put pressure on the candidates spokesmen to explain the sudden
changes. For example, did a foreign crisis bolster the President?
Experts compare watching the daily tracking to watching the daily Dow Jones average
and explaining, after the fact, what made it go up today or go down.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Would it help to report an average the last three days' polls?
(2) The margin of error for the Gallup poll is 4 percentage points. How much of
a gap could there be between two successive days' results for the polls still to
be within their margin of errors?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Misreading the gender gap.
New York Times 17 Sept. 1996, A23
Carol Tavris
The author lists several examples of supposed gender gaps which disappear when a relevant
trait is controlled for.
Women are more likely to believe in horoscopes and psychics. (Not if you control
for the number of math and science classes a person has had. What appears to be a
gender gap is a science gap.)
Men are more likely than women to express anger directly and abusively. (Not if you
control for the status of the individuals involved. What appears to be a gender gap
is a power gap.)
Now everyone is talking about the gender gap in the current Presidential race. According
to the most recent New York Times/CBS News poll, women prefer Clinton to Dole by
61% to 33%.
Conservatives explain this gap by saying that women tend to be more sentimental, more
risk-averse and less competitive than men, while liberals claim that women are more
compassionate and less aggressive than men, and thus attracted to the party that
will help the weakest members of society.
Tavris rejects both of these explanations claiming "affluent women have never shown
an especially tender-hearted sympathy toward their sisters in poverty" and making
similar remarks about the other supposed differences.
She suggests that the gender gap in the political situation is largely an experience
gap. If their husband has left them they may have been saved by welfare. More women
than men are taking care of aging, infirm parents. More single mothers than single
fathers are taking care of children on their own etc. Tavris remarks: "For women to
perceive the Democrats more responsive than the Republicans to these concerns is
neither sentimental nor irrational. It stems from self interest."
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Can you think of other gender gaps that might disappear when you control for
a relevant factor?
(2) Do you agree with the author's explanation for the gender gap in politics?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Michael Olinick provided the following story, which he heard on NPR and tracked down
the transcript on Nexis/Lexis.
NPR Weekend Edition Sunday (10:00 am ET)
18 August 1996, Transcript # 1189-6
The following puzzle was read on the air by Will Shortz: ...This week's challenge
is a logical puzzle by Terry Stickels [sp], who has written a 365-day puzzle calendar
for 1997 called Mind Bending Puzzles. There are four colored balls in a bag; two
red, one black, and one blue. If you draw two balls at random, and then you're told that
one of them is red, what is the likelihood that the other ball is also red?"
Listeners were encouraged to mail solutions on a post-card to Weekend Edition Sunday
Puzzle at NPR.
DISCUSSION QUESTION: What is the probability?
------------------------
NPR Weekend Edition Sunday (10:00 am ET)
25 August 1996, Transcript # 1190-6
LIANE HANSEN, Host: This is Weekend Edition; I'm Liane Hansen. And joining us is
puzzle master Will Shortz...Well, I tell you, we are all confounded by last week's
challenge...We've been talking about it pretty much up until the point of this broadcast.
SHORTZ: Well, it stumped a lot of people. It was from a 1997 calendar by Terry Stickels
[sp] called Mind-Bending Puzzles....The likelihood is 20 percent, or one chance in
five, and here's why. There are only six combinations of balls; red one and red
two, red one and black, red one and blue, red two and black, red two and blue, and black
and blue. Now, since you were told that one of the balls was red, you can ignore
this last combination. There are five combinations left that have red in them, only
one of which has two reds, so the odds are one in five.
LIANE HANSEN: We had about 600 entries this past week, in spite of the-the challenge,
and the fact that a lot of us are probability impaired...
Our winner is Susan Ehrhardt, and she lives in Oswego, New York; and, Susan, first
of all, I have to thank you very much for giving us some visual aids to Will's puzzle.
You actually drew the combinations that came up. Nice going, Susan.....What do
you do there in Oswego?
SUSAN EHRHARDT: I'm a veterinarian.
LIANE HANSEN: Oh. How long did it take you to come up with the answer to this puzzle?
SUSAN EHRHARDT: Well, I've had statistics courses, so it's-it was a second, so-
LIANE HANSEN: A second, she says [laughs].
SUSAN EHRHARDT: But I'm not probability impaired.
DISCUSSION QUESTION: A second?!
--------------------
NPR Weekend Edition Sunday (10:00 am ET)
1 September 1996, Transcript # 1191-5
LIANE HANSEN: .....you know, Will, people are still talking about those balls--
WILL SHORTZ: Uh-huh.
LIANE HANSEN: -from two weeks ago.
WILL SHORTZ: I- I had a feeling that would happen.
LIANE HANSEN: So what are we gonna do?
WILL SHORTZ: Well, I have written a fuller explanation of the solution to that statistics
puzzle from two weeks ago, and you can get it by sending us a self-addressed stamped
envelope, or by going to the Weekend Edition Web page on the Internet, either way.
The URL is http://www.npr.org/programs/wesun/puzzle_archive.html
Included there is a response to questions by many listeners who wondered why the answer
isn't 1 in 3. They reason that, given one of the balls being red, the other ball
by elimination must be red, black, or blue. Thus, the odds of the second ball being
red are 1 in 3.
DISCUSSION QUESTION: What is wrong with the argument for 1 in 3?
Editor's comment: This kind of problem also interested Lewis Carroll whose Pillow-Problem
number 5 was:
A bag contains one counter, known to be either
white or black. A white counter is put in, the
bag shaken, and a counter drawn out, which proves
to be white. What is now the chance of drawing
a white counter?
DISCUSSION QUESTION:
And what is the answer to Lewis Carroll's problem?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Ask Marilyn.
Parade Magazine, 22 September 1996, p.20
Marilyn vos Savant
Blaise Pascal, a scientist and philosopher,
posed a decision problem for atheists, sometimes
called 'Pascal's Wager.' If you were an atheist,
how would you respond?
Glenn Niblock,
San Diego, Calif.
Pascal's argument is that, if God does not exist, one loses relatively little by believing
in him, whereas there is a tremendous loss in failing to believe in him if he really
does exist. Weighing these alternatives, a rational man
should gamble that God exits.
Marilyn asserts that the wager contains two important assumptions: (1) God exists
and (2) If he exists, believing in him brings eternal life. She notes that the argument
can be adapted to reason that a religion that promises eternal life to the faithful
is better than one which promises, for example, only a few days of life after death.
And a religion that promises eternal life plus some other benefits would look even
more attractive. In other words, the best bet would be to join the religion that
makes the most promises.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS: Do you agree with Marilyn?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Boys' club.
Athletic Business, May 1996, p9
Rick Berg
The article is a bit dated, but pertains to a current issue at Middlebury, where our
athletic director is retiring and we are conducting a national search for his replacement.
One of the members of the search committee forwarded this article to me (Bill Peterson) for comment. The article notes that most Division I-A Athletic Directors
(ADs) are male, former players and former coaches, even though college presidents
report that none of these credentials are requirements for the job.
Toni Wyatt, a professor of sport management at Appalachian State University, surveyed
Division A-I presidents to learn what they think is important in an AD, and compared
the results with the profiles of those who actually get hired. Some of his findings:
- Of presidents who think coaching experience is important, 75% say the sport coached
is irrelevant. Yet 60% of ADs who were coaches coached football.
- Of presidents who say athletic experience is important, 71.4% say the level played
at is unimportant, and 92.6% say the sport played is irrelevant. Yet 79.1% of ADs
played at the Division I level; 65.1% played football.
- 28.3% of presidents say previous experience at that institution is unimportant.
Yet 55.1% of ADs had previous experience at their institution as a player coach or
administrator.
Prof. Wyatt speculates that football experience is more important to presidents than
they might like to admit. The survey also finds the following demographic profile:
96.2% of presidents and 98% of ADs are male; 92.5% and 97.9% are white.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
(1) Do the data mean that presidents are simply saying one thing and doing another?
What other information would you like to have to evaluate the hiring practices?
(2) What do you make of the football connection?
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
NOTE:
The Fifth International Conference on Teaching Statistics - ICOTS-5
Place Nanyang Technological University, Singapore,
Dates June 21 - 26, 1998.
Theme Statistical Education - Expanding the Network
Contacts
Chair IPC Brian Phillips (bphillips@swin.edu.au
Fax + 61 9819 0821)
Chair LOC Teck-Wong Soon (twsoon@singstat.gov.sg)
Singapore contact Lionel Pereira-Mendoza (pereiraml@am.nie.ac.sg)
WWW site www.nie.ac.sg:8000/~wwwmath/icots.html
CALL for PAPERS
If you are interested in presenting a paper at ICOTS-5 please submit an abstract,
300 - 500 words, of the paper you would like to be considered as soon as possible
but before Dec. 18, 1996 to the relevant topic convener whose name and email is listed
below or the IPC Chair. You will then be put in touch with the appropriate session organizer.
Note: Because of the limited number of speakers who can be accepted for each session,
people whose abstracts are not accepted for the session they nominate may be referred
to organizers of other relevant sessions and/or the contributed paper or poster sessions.
Topics
1. Statistical education at the school level (Elementary level, secondary level, teacher
training, local teachers) Lionel Pereira-Mendoza pereiraml@am.nie.ac.sg
2. Statistical education at the post-secondary level (Introductory statistics, mathematical
statistics, design and analysis of experiments, regression and correlation, Bayesian
methods, categorical data analysis, sample survey design and analysis) Richard Scheaffer scheaffe@stat.ufl.edu
3. Statistical education for people in the workplace (Statistical consultancy, continuing
education, distance education, total quality) Kerstin Vannman kerstin.vannman@ies.luth.se
4. Statistical education and the wider society (Statistical Societies, statistical
literacy, publications, legal contexts, journalists, informed society) Anne Hawkins
ash@maths.nott.ac.uk
5. An international perspective of statistical education (African region,Asian region,
Spanish speaking , Other developing regions,) James Ntozi isae@mukla.gn.apc.org
6. Research in teaching statistics
(Junior levels, senior school levels, post-secondary levels, probability) Joan Garfield
jbg@maroon.tc.umn.edu
7. The role of technology in the teaching of statistics (Software design,teaching
experiments, graphics calculators, visualization, research,multi-media and WWW)
Rolf Biehler rolf.biehler@post.uni-bielefeld.de
8. Other determinants and developments in statistical education (Cultural/historical
factors, learning factors, assessment, gender factors, projects/competitions)
Guiseppe Cicchitelli pino@stat.unipg.it
9. Contributed papers
Shir-Ming Shen hrntssm@hkucc.hku.hk
10 Poster sessions
Peng Yee Lee leepy@am.nie.ac.sg
Note: Anyone who wants to run a special session such as a special interest group discussion,
a demonstration/training session should contact the IPC Chair for consideration.
<<<========<<
>>>>>==============>
Please send comments and suggestions for articles to
jlsnell@dartmouth.edu.
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!
CHANCE News 5.11
(8 September 1996 to 8 October 1996)
!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!